Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Orienteering experts report more proficient spatial processing and memory across adulthood

  • Emma E. Waddington ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    waddinge@mcmaster.ca

    Affiliation Faculty of Science, Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada

  • Jennifer J. Heisz

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Science, Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada

Abstract

The closest surrogate to hunter-gather activity is the sport of orienteering, which naturally and simultaneously combines high-intensity interval exercise with navigation. Although human cognition can be improved across the lifespan through exercise and cognitive training, interventions like orienteering may be especially effective because they resemble activities engaged in by prehistoric humans during evolution. The present study tested whether orienteering experts have better hippocampal-dependent cognitive function than active, non-orienteering controls. One-hundred and fifty-eight healthy adults between the ages of 18 and 87 years old with varying experience in orienteering (none, intermediate, advanced, elite) reported on their spatial processing, spatial memory and episodic memory using the Navigational Strategy Questionnaire and the Survey of Autobiographical Memory. Orienteering experts reported greater use of allocentric and egocentric spatial processing and better spatial memory than controls. In contrast, episodic memory was not associated with orienteering expertise. Notably, the significant effects of orienteering on spatial cognition remained even after controlling for age, sex, and physical activity, suggesting that orienteering may be an effective intervention to prevent age-related cognitive decline in spatial navigation and memory.

Introduction

The human brain evolved under the harsh environmental conditions of our evolutionary past, and modern life may lack the specific cognitive and physical challenges that the brain needs to thrive [1,2]. Consider navigation, for example; humans are no longer required to hunt and gather food to survive and now rely on passive transport and Global Positioning System (GPS) navigation [35]. While GPS navigation is convenient, it requires minimal cognitive stimulation, and its long-term use can impair spatial memory skills during self-guided navigation tasks such as map reading [3]. Without consistent use of our navigational aptness, we may risk losing aspects of our spatial processing abilities and their supporting neural architecture. Consequently, this could impact various aspects of human cognition [2,6].

In healthy aging, there is unequal atrophy across the human brain. The hippocampus atrophies more than the parietal regions [7], impairing memory and particular spatial processing abilities more than others [8]. For example, allocentric spatial processing, a navigational strategy from an object-based or third-person perspective (relative to the environment), is mediated by the hippocampus and is more impaired with advancing age [8,9]. In contrast, egocentric spatial processing, a navigational strategy based on a first-person perspective (relative to the body), is mediated by the caudate nucleus and parietal regions and is less impaired with advancing age [8,10]. Successful navigation requires flexibly switching and combining these different spatial strategies. Here, spatial information is translated from its stored form in an allocentric reference frame into an egocentric reference frame to use while navigating [11,12]. However, the age-related losses in allocentric processing lead older adults to rely more on egocentric processing [13,14]. Wayfinding can also be achieved using a route or procedural strategy, a less robust, non-spatial navigation technique that relies on memorizing a list of landmarks as cues for turning directions [15] and can be compromised by aging [16]. Taken together, older adults tend to experience more navigational problems that may ultimately limit their mobility [17].

Compared to healthy aging, age-related impairments in general cognition, such as mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease, are accompanied by more significant hippocampal degeneration [18,19], further impairing their allocentric spatial processing and wayfinding abilities [20,21]. At worse, the individual may be unable to create, store or use mental maps of new environments needed to navigate [2224], a multifaceted condition known as topographical disorientation [25,26]. Even self-reported complaints about spatial navigation abilities are higher in those with mild cognitive impairments and Alzheimer’s disease than controls [27]. Evidence suggests that early signs of topographical disorientation may be a predictor of general cognitive impairment later in life, whereby healthy middle-aged adults with risk factors for dementia also tend to have poorer allocentric spatial processing than their peers [21]. It follows that interventions designed to strengthen allocentric spatial processing may also help stave off general cognitive decline and topographical disorientation [28].

According to the Adaptive Capacity Model [2], coupling navigation-based tasks with movement may be an effective mechanism to enhance cognition in domains that often deteriorate with age. The model theorizes that the human brain may be more receptive to interventions that resemble the hunter-gatherer activities that humans engaged in when the human brain evolved, such as memorizing spatial routes while navigating [2,29]. Although physical exercise alone increases cognition [3032] and neurogenesis [3336] and can add to the benefits of cognitive training [3741], many interventions have participants perform exercise training and cognitive training asynchronously. This separation may, however, lessen the evolutionary semblance of the training and therefore diminish the associated cognitive gains. Indeed, a recent meta-analysis found synchronous training to be slightly (though not significantly) more effective at improving cognition in older adults than sequential training [42]. We propose that the effect size of synchronous over asynchronous training may depend on how seamlessly the cognitive and exercise training components are combined.

The sport of orienteering seamlessly and simultaneously integrates movement with navigation that is prehistorically familiar to the brain and, therefore, is the closest surrogate to hunter-gatherer activities [2]. The goal of orienteering is to navigate by running as quickly as possible over unfamiliar terrain through a series of checkpoints using only a topographical map and a compass [43]. The most skillful orienteers must efficiently switch between various mental tasks, making quick decisions while moving across the terrain at a rapid pace [44,45]. The unique thing about orienteering is that it requires active navigation, encompassing quick and recurrent transitions between allocentric and egocentric spatial processing in real-time, which GPS systems have engineered out of modern life [3,6]. The frequent and intermittent engagement of allocentric processing by orienteers may help them counteract the typical decline that occurs with aging. However, little research has assessed the effects of real-world orienteering experience on hippocampal-related functioning. One study found that expert orienteers with over eight years of experience in the sport outperformed non-orienteering controls on tasks involving object-based mental rotation [46]. Another research group had 15 students complete VR orienteering races over six days and observed improvements in the same object-based mental rotation task but did not compare the effects to real-world orienteering experience [47]. They also failed to include the physical stimulus of exercise, which we would argue makes orienteering so unique to the field of exercise cognition. A third study had 24 college students complete a 12-week real-world orienteering intervention at a moderate exercise intensity and observed improvements on a computer-based two-dimensional visuospatial working memory task, however, the improvements were only observed in male participants [48]. While these findings are promising, more work is needed.

The present study examined the effects of orienteering, which simultaneously combines movement and navigation, on hippocampal-related functioning in adults aged 18 to 87 years old with varying orienteering skill levels (intermediate, advanced, elite) plus a physically active non-orienteer control group. We included a physically active control group who had little to no orienteering experience to assess the effect of orienteering expertise over and above the benefits of physical activity engagement without navigation. All participants reported on their spatial processing, spatial memory and episodic memory using the Navigational Strategy Questionnaire and the Survey of Autobiographical Memory. We expected participants with more orienteering experience to report greater use of hippocampal-related allocentric spatial processing. We also examined whether orienteering experience would transfer to other spatial and episodic memory domains that also depend on the hippocampus. Finally, we examined whether the benefits of orienteering on spatial processing and memory remained after controlling for the effects of age, sex, and physical activity level.

Methods

Participants

One hundred and fifty-eight healthy adults (77 men, 81 women; age M (SD) = 48 (± 20) years, range = 18–87 years) completed the online survey. Orienteers (N = 114) of subjectively rated intermediate to elite skill level who regularly participate in the sport were recruited from orienteering clubs from around the world using the following inclusion criteria: English language proficiency, participation in foot-orienteering five times or more from intermediate to elite skill level, not exceeding moderate weekly alcohol and/or drug use (≤ 10 servings of alcohol per week [49], ≤ 7 days per week using electronic or regular cigarettes, and infrequent (weekly or less) use of marijuana [50] or other recreational drugs) and having no diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment. To assess the effect of orienteering compared to other forms of aerobic exercise that are less cognitively and navigationally demanding, physically active individuals (N = 44) from aerobic-based sports organizations (i.e., running, cycling, etc.) from around the world were recruited into the control group using the same inclusion criteria. Considering previous work that grouped participants as “orienteers” if they had a minimum of six 2-hour sessions of formal orienteering training [51], control participants in this study were included if they had either no orienteering experience (zero exposures) or an insignificant level of exposure that would not impact their navigational aptitudes (i.e., four or fewer single-session exposures).

All participants provided written informed consent through the online questionnaire and were able to enter a draw to receive an honorarium of fifty Canadian dollars. This project was approved by the McMaster Research Ethics Board (# 2394).

Procedure

All participants completed an online questionnaire (LimeSurvey Software) consisting of a demographic questionnaire, the Navigation Strategy questionnaire [15], and the Survey of Autobiographical Memory [52], outlined below.

Participants in the orienteering group completed additional questions based on their orienteering experience, which consisted of a series of evidence-based questions created ad hoc through consultation with orienteering experts to assess common orienteering concepts (see S1 Appendix) [45]. Orienteering skill can be ascertained by the difficulty of courses typically navigated and the frequency and magnitude of deviations from the optimal or intended route. In orienteering, these deviations are referred to as “mistakes”, which become smaller and less frequent with increasing skill levels [45]. As such, responses from the second question of this survey were used to group orienteers into the appropriate skill level (i.e., intermediate: “I can navigate well both on and off trails but often have large issues”; advanced: “I can navigate well off trails with few available features and seldom make large mistakes”; elite: “I hardly ever make errors in my navigation and feel confident in many kinds of terrain”) whereby those with greater skill level also tended to have reported a greater number of years of orienteering experience.

Demographic information.

Demographic information, including the year of birth, sex, nationality, employment status, and education, was collected. All participants completed the Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour Questionnaire (PASB-Q) from the Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology [53]. Total aerobic physical activity from the PASB-Q was used to estimate the participant’s weekly exercise engagement by multiplying the average number of active days per week by the average length of active time (minutes/week).

The navigational strategy questionnaire.

The Navigational Strategy Questionnaire (NSQ) [15] was used to evaluate the navigational tendencies of all participants. In the NSQ, participants used a 5-point Likert scale to rank 44 items which described concepts related to one of three navigational strategies to receive an average score for each strategy. The use of hippocampal-dependent allocentric spatial processing was assessed with questions such as “I usually attempt to mentally represent route segments, turns and their spatial relationships from a top-down aerial perspective” (Question 14, NSQ). The use of egocentric spatial processing indicates that individuals approach navigational tasks from a first-person view and can: “… visualize [the] environment in the form of a 3D spatial layout …” (Question 31, NSQ). Though egocentric navigation is not dependent on hippocampal processing [10], the hippocampus aids in transitioning between egocentric and allocentric spatial processing when navigating. The use of the procedural strategy assesses an individual’s reliance on non-spatial methods for navigation with a preference for: “…following directions with descriptions of landmarks at turning points rather than using a map” (Question 18, NSQ).

The survey of autobiographical memory.

The Survey of Autobiographical Memory (SAM) Questionnaire is a 26-item subjective memory survey that gives a general self-reported measure of episodic, semantic, spatial, and future memory [52]. Each item is answered using a five-point Likert scale, and scores are weighted appropriately and summed for each memory type. In this project, we analyzed spatial and episodic memory, with statements such as: “In general, my ability to navigate is better than most of my family/friends” (Question 1, SAM) for spatial memory and “When I remember events, I remember a lot of details” (Question 6, SAM) for episodic memory.

Statistical analysis

All data were analyzed using SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, version 28.0; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). Raw data from the online questionnaire software were screened for missing cells (unanswered questions). Normality was assessed using the Shapiro-Wilk test and using visual inspection of histograms. Not all variables showed normal distribution, but all showed strong linearity when examined visually using normal Q-Q plots. As such, non-parametric tests were used.

Kruskal-Wallis H tests were used to compare mean rank differences between the level of orienteering experience with demographic factors (age, physical activity, sex, years of orienteering experience) and cognition (allocentric spatial processing, egocentric spatial processing, procedural navigation, spatial memory, episodic memory). Post hoc tests were completed using Dunn’s pairwise tests for all four pairs of orienteering experience level groups. Spearman’s rank correlations were also used to assess the associations of orienteering experience with demographic factors and cognition, and partial Spearman’s correlations were used to evaluate whether the relationships between orienteering experience and cognition remained after controlling for age, sex, and physical activity. All statistical tests were computed using a 95% confidence interval and an alpha criterion of 0.05.

Results

Participants

Participant demographics by group can be found in Table 1. Elite orienteers were younger in age (H(3) = 15.66, p = .001; ε2 = .10) and more physically active (H(3) = 14.59, p = .002; ε2 = .09) than intermediate or advanced orienteers but only differed from controls in physical activity levels (p < .05). The sex of participants differed between groups (H(3) = 13.41, p = .004; ε2 = .09), with a higher male-female ratio among elite orienteers than intermediates or controls (p < .05). Although education level was relatively balanced across the groups, especially in post-secondary and post-graduate studies, control participants differed compared to elite and advanced orienteers and elites differed compared to intermediates (p < .05). Subjectively rated orienteering level was significantly associated with the number of years of orienteering experience (rs(158) = .78, p < .001), where 47% (N = 34/72) of advanced and elite orienteers combined reported 20 or more years of experience.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and average outcome measures across groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0280435.t001

Egocentric, allocentric and procedural spatial processing

Fig 1 depicts the large positive effect of orienteering on self-reported egocentric processing (H(3) = 31.16, p < .001; ε2 = .20), allocentric processing (H(3) = 17.98, p < .001; ε2 = .11), as well as the negative effect of orienteering on procedural navigation (H(3) = 44.10, p < .001; ε2 = .28). Post hoc tests using Dunn’s pairwise test indicated that advanced and elite orienteers reported engaging in more allocentric and egocentric processing, and less procedural processing than controls or intermediate participants (all p < .05). There were no differences between control and intermediate, or between advanced and elite orienteers for either allocentric or egocentric spatial processing (p > .05); however, there was a difference between intermediate orienteers and controls for procedural processing, where intermediates indicated less reliance on this strategy (p < .05).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Average score on NSQ spatial processing measure across groups.

A boxplot demonstrating mean, minimum, and max values for A) egocentric spatial processing, B) allocentric spatial processing, and C) procedural processing. Error bars reflect SD.* reflects p < .05, ** reflects p < .01, *** reflects p < .001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0280435.g001

Spatial and episodic memory

Fig 2 depicts the large effect of orienteering on self-reported spatial memory (H(3) = 45.51, p < .001; ε2 = .29). Elite and advanced orienteers reported better spatial memory than intermediate orienteers and non-orienteers. Post hoc tests using Dunn’s pairwise tests indicated that advanced and elite orienteers had greater ratings of spatial memory than either control or intermediate orienteers (p < .001). There was no difference between advanced and elite or between control and intermediate participants (p > .05). In contrast, orienteering experience was not associated with self-reported episodic memory (H (3) = 2.75, p = .433; ε2 = .02) at any group level.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Average score on SAM autobiographical memory measure across groups.

A boxplot demonstrating mean, minimum, and max values for A) spatial memory, B) episodic memory. Error bars reflect SD. *** reflects p < .001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0280435.g002

Covariates of age, sex, and physical activity

Consistent with the Kruskal-Wallis H tests, we observed significant Spearman rank correlates, revealing that greater orienteering experience was positively associated with egocentric spatial processing (rs(158) = .38, p < .001), allocentric spatial processing (rs(158) = .28, p < .001) and spatial memory (rs(158) = .50 p < .001) but negatively associated with procedural navigation (rs(158) = -.53, p < .001). Participants with greater orienteering experience also tended to be younger (rs(158) = -.17, p = .038), male (rs(158) = -.26, p < .001), and more physically active (rs(158) = .17, p = .033); however, entering age, sex, and physical activity as covariates in the partial correlation analyses did not alter the associations between orienteering experience, spatial processing, or memory. Table 2 shows the correlational matrix of the relationships between orienteering experience with all facets of the NSQ and SAM while also controlling for age, sex, and weekly physical activity level.

thumbnail
Table 2. Correlation matrix with age, sex, and weekly physical activity as control variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0280435.t002

Discussion

The present study examined whether more experience in the sport of orienteering was associated with greater self-reported use of hippocampal-dependent spatial processing tendencies. Orienteers were expected to have higher self-reported allocentric spatial processing abilities because the sport encompasses the utilization of this process more than in modern life. Interestingly, elite and advanced orienteers reported greater use of allocentric and egocentric spatial processing and had higher subjective spatial memory than physically active controls with little to no orienteering experience. The results remained significant even after controlling for age, sex, and physical activity, suggesting that engagement in the sport of orienteering may be beneficial to essential aspects of spatial processing and memory across the lifespan.

The observed association between orienteering and spatial processing is particularly impressive given that the age range of our sample was 18 to 87 years old, and the end of this age range is typically marred by marked declines in allocentric processing [8]. Of course, this is a cross-sectional study, and we must include the fact that those with better spatial processing may be merely self-selecting into the sport. At least one study found evidence for trait-like differences in expert orienteers who reported experiencing more pleasure from exploring novel terrains than beginner orienteers or controls [46], and 60% of the orienteers in this study indicated monthly usage of a map and compass outside of the sport of orienteering (see S1 Table). Yet, the alternative is worth considering—that the engagement in orienteering may be neural protective to spatial domains of cognition, including both spatial processing and spatial memory. This interpretation is consistent with prior work demonstrating the effectiveness of short-term orienteering training on visuospatial working memory [48] and longitudinal data demonstrating that greater GPS use is associated with a steeper decline in hippocampal-dependent spatial memory [3]. Taken together, our results and others suggest that aging-related decline in spatial memory may not only be related to aging per se but also to losing what we do not use (i.e., “use it or lose it”). Unlike most modern-day dwellers, orienteers regularly engage in active navigation, which may mitigate their cognitive decline as they age.

The Adaptive Capacity Model would attribute the large benefit of orienteering on cognition to the evolutionary semblance of the sport and the brain’s receptiveness to such activities [2]. The quick and recurrent transitions between allocentric and egocentric spatial processing that orienteers engage in may also play an important role. An orienteer’s successful navigation and route recall depend on their ability to translate and integrate allocentric and egocentric representations while synchronously navigating and moving throughout the surrounding environment [11,12]. The routine use of this allocentric-to-egocentric spatial translation could explain why the orienteers perceived their abilities to be superior in hippocampal-dependent allocentric spatial processing, egocentric spatial processing, and spatial memory. In the context of aging-related changes, spatial memory is hypothesized to provide a “scaffold” for episodic memories [54], and the inability to reinstate a spatial scaffold from long-term memory is postulated to underlie memory impairments in Alzheimer’s disease [55]. Given that engagement in the sport of orienteering was associated with better spatial memory reports across the lifespan, it follows that orienteering may be a viable intervention strategy to reduce the risk of Alzheimer’s disease.

That said, orienteers did not rate episodic memory differently relative to the active control group. This may be surprising given that spatial memory provides crucial information for episodic memory concerning “where” the episode (or event) took place. However, several things may account for this. First, the control group was very physically active, and physical activity, especially when done at a high intensity, enhances episodic memory across the lifespan [38,56]. While the orienteers in our sample were frequently physically active, it is possible that their activity was not vigorous enough to facilitate neurogenic adaptations needed to enhance episodic memory. To further investigate the impacts of orienteering on episodic memory, future work should include measurements of exercise intensity or compare orienteers to a sedentary control group, which is minimally examined in the current literature.

Alternatively, adding navigation to physical activity may not boost episodic memory over and above the benefits of physical activity alone. Episodic memory can rely on non-spatial elements, including people and objects [54], and the orienteers were less likely to use non-spatial procedural navigation strategies. This may suggest a potential tradeoff where orienteers favour spatial over non-spatial information processing resulting in no net gain for episodic memory.

With respect to orienteering skills, elite and advanced orienteers perceived their spatial cognition to be superior to that of intermediate orienteers even after controlling for age, sex, and physical activity, though advanced and elite groups did not often differ. This can be seen in the positive association between higher orienteering skill and the number of years of experience, where almost half of the advanced and elite orienteers indicated 20 or more years of experience, whereas the majority of intermediate orienteers reported less than 20 years of experience (S1 Table). Most advanced and elite orienteers also reported training and racing more frequently in orienteering compared to the intermediates (S1 Table). These results suggest that prolonged and consistent practice of orienteering is required for cognitive gains. Another point to consider is that a high proportion of elite orienteers in our sample were European. Although people living in Europe tend to have relatively good navigational abilities compared to other countries across the globe, this may be especially true in countries like Finland, Norway, and Sweden because they include orienteering in school curriculums and tend to have higher success rates at international competitions [57]. However, in the context of “use it or lose it,” even active rehearsal of lower-level orienteering skills would be expected to strengthen unused neural pathways. Ideally, we would have recruited beginner-level orienteers to test this hypothesis; however, none of our participants were at that level. Nonetheless, the “use it or lose it” hypothesis is supported by the prior observations that short-term real-world orienteering training results in spatial processing gains [48] and that individuals who rely less on GPS have better navigation skills [3]. Longitudinal interventions are needed to determine how much navigational practice is required to alter evolutionarily relevant but neglected navigational skills.

While this study makes an important contribution to understanding the benefit of orienteering for human cognition, it is not without its limitations. This virtual study allowed us to recruit a large and diverse sample of participants from across the globe, and we used self-reported assessments to explore multiple cognitive domains relating to hippocampal functioning in a quick and accessible way. Although self-reports do not always correspond well to objective measures of cognition [58,59], physical tasks tend to be less impacted by metacognitive inaccuracies than cognitive tasks [60] and the self-reported assessments used here show good corresponded with objective measures [15,52,61]. Furthermore, when it comes to Alzheimer’s and related dementias, subjective memory complaints are often used as an early indicator of increased risk, even in the absence of overt memory impairment [27]. That said, as future research continues to examine how orienteering expertise impacts objective cognitive performance, our self-reported results suggest that certain cognitive domains, namely, spatial navigational strategies and spatial memory, should be further compared with objective measures.

As the research on orienteering for cognition evolves, it will be necessary to operationally define what it means to be an “orienteer” and what factors should be examined when classifying one’s level of orienteering expertise. In this study, we used an ad hoc questionnaire asking participants to report on their orienteering experience (see S1 Appendix). Although our classification of orienteering expertise was subjective, the self-reported rankings correlated well with the participants’ perceived skill level and years of orienteering experience. Based on prior literature, the least amount of experience needed to qualify as an “orienteer” was six to eight 2-hour formal orienteering training sessions [51]. Another prior study used the criteria of “more than five years of experience” to classify beginner orienteers [46]. Most of the orienteers in this study fell beyond this criterion, with more than one-third of participants reporting over 20 years of experience. Moreover, although we included sex as a covariate in our analyses and it did not impact our results, males and females often approach navigational tasks differently [62], and one prior study of 24 participants found that only males benefited from a 12-week orienteering training program whereas females did not [48]. Moving forward, it is important for future research to consider sex differences in the effects of orienteering on cognition.

Finally, although we controlled for many factors, we did not control for the participants’ prior experience with VR or video gaming. One question (See S1 Appendix: Q5) evaluated the orienteers’ weekly use of orienteering maps without being physically active (i.e., map making or playing orienteering virtual reality (VR) videogames), and only 21% of the orienteers (N = 24/114) engaged in these tasks for more than 1 hour per week (S1 Table). To date, many interventions designed to boost navigational abilities have relied on navigational training using VR [24,63]. For example, a recent study found that playing a 3D navigation-based videogame for five consecutive days improved both allocentric spatial processing and spatial memory [63]. However, based on our understanding of the Adaptive Capacity Model [2], coupling VR training with movement may replicate real-world navigation and provide greater cognitive benefits. This is supported by two studies that found that when coupling VR spatial navigation training with treadmill walking over four months, expected age-related atrophy of the hippocampus could be mitigated [64] and decreases in cortical thickness were prevented [65]. Given the promising effects of VR navigation training on cognition, one would expect the cognitive benefits to be augmented by real-world navigation training, such as orienteering. Future work related to orienteering should compare real-world navigation with VR navigation while also controlling for other factors that may affect spatial processing capacity, including video game use, type and cognitive demand of occupation, and sports engagement.

Conclusion

In conclusion, expert orienteers reported greater reliance on allocentric and egocentric spatial processing abilities and had better perceived spatial memory capacity. This is the first study to demonstrate a link between engagement in the sport of orienteering and hippocampal-dependent processes, and it suggests that training in the sport of orienteering may be more beneficial than physical activity alone. Although this cross-sectional study cannot speak to the causality of these associations, these novel results point to the sport of orienteering as a viable brain training regime to combat age-related cognitive decline.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Orienteering experience questionnaire.

The above questions comprised the Orienteering Experience Segment on the administered online questionnaire used in this study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0280435.s001

(PDF)

S1 Table. Summarized responses to the orienteering experience questionnaire.

Bolded values indicate largest percentage within group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0280435.s002

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the NeuroFit Lab team and those who helped explore this novel area of research.

References

  1. 1. Lieberman DE. Is Exercise Really Medicine? An Evolutionary Perspective: Curr Sports Med Rep. 2015;14(4):313–9. pmid:26166056
  2. 2. Raichlen DA, Alexander GE. Adaptive Capacity: An Evolutionary Neuroscience Model Linking Exercise, Cognition, and Brain Health. Trends Neurosci. 2017 Jul;40(7):408–21. pmid:28610948
  3. 3. Dahmani L, Bohbot VD. Habitual use of GPS negatively impacts spatial memory during self-guided navigation. Sci Rep. 2020 Dec;10(1):6310. pmid:32286340
  4. 4. Ishikawa T. Satellite Navigation and Geospatial Awareness: Long-Term Effects of Using Navigation Tools on Wayfinding and Spatial Orientation. Prof Geogr. 2019 Apr 3;71(2):197–209.
  5. 5. McKinlay R. Technology: Use or lose our navigation skills. Nature. 2016 Mar 31;531(7596):573–5. pmid:27029262
  6. 6. Javadi AH, Emo B, Howard LR, Zisch FE, Yu Y, Knight R, et al. Hippocampal and prefrontal processing of network topology to simulate the future. Nat Commun. 2017 Mar 21;8:14652. pmid:28323817
  7. 7. Fraser MA, Shaw ME, Cherbuin N. A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal hippocampal atrophy in healthy human ageing. NeuroImage. 2015 May;112:364–74. pmid:25800208
  8. 8. Colombo D, Serino S, Tuena C, Pedroli E, Dakanalis A, Cipresso P, et al. Egocentric and allocentric spatial reference frames in aging: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017 Sep;80:605–21. pmid:28760627
  9. 9. Ekstrom AD, Kahana MJ, Caplan JB, Fields TA, Isham EA, Newman EL, et al. Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature. 2003 Sep;425(6954):184–8. pmid:12968182
  10. 10. Cook D, Kesner RP. Caudate nucleus and memory for egocentric localization. Behav Neural Biol. 1988 May;49(3):332–43. pmid:3408445
  11. 11. Byrne P, Becker S, Burgess N. Remembering the past and imagining the future: A neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol Rev. 2007 Apr;114(2):340–75. pmid:17500630
  12. 12. Serino S, Giuseppe R. What is the role of spatial processing in the decline of episodic memory in Alzheimer’s disease? The “mental frame syncing” hypothesis. 2014 Mar 10;6:33.
  13. 13. Moffat SD, Elkins W, Resnick SM. Age differences in the neural systems supporting human allocentric spatial navigation. Neurobiol Aging. 2006 Jul;27(7):965–72. pmid:15982787
  14. 14. Rodgers MK, Sindone JA, Moffat SD. Effects of age on navigation strategy. Neurobiol Aging. 2012 Jan;33(1):202.e15-202.e22. pmid:20832911
  15. 15. Zhong JY, Kozhevnikov M. Relating allocentric and egocentric survey-based representations to the self-reported use of a navigation strategy of egocentric spatial updating. J Environ Psychol. 2016;46:154–75.
  16. 16. Ramanoël S, Durteste M, Bécu M, Habas C, Arleo A. Differential Brain Activity in Regions Linked to Visuospatial Processing During Landmark-Based Navigation in Young and Healthy Older Adults. Front Hum Neurosci. 2020 Oct 29;14:552111. pmid:33240060
  17. 17. Burns PC. Navigation and the Mobility of Older Drivers. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1999 Jan 1;54B(1):S49–55. pmid:9934402
  18. 18. Barnes J, Bartlett JW, van de Pol LA, Loy CT, Scahill RI, Frost C, et al. A meta-analysis of hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2009 Nov;30(11):1711–23. pmid:18346820
  19. 19. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol (Berl). 1991 Sep;82(4):239–59. pmid:1759558
  20. 20. Nedelska Z, Andel R, Laczó J, Vlcek K, Horinek D, Lisy J, et al. Spatial navigation impairment is proportional to right hippocampal volume. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012 Feb 14;109(7):2590–4. pmid:22308496
  21. 21. Ritchie K, Carrière I, Howett D, Su L, Hornberger M, O’Brien JT, et al. Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Processing in Middle-Aged Adults at High Risk of Late-Onset Alzheimer’s Disease: The PREVENT Dementia Study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018 Sep 11;65(3):885–96. pmid:30103333
  22. 22. Boccia M, Di Vita A, Diana S, Margiotta R, Imbriano L, Rendace L, et al. Is Losing One’s Way a Sign of Cognitive Decay? Topographical Memory Deficit as an Early Marker of Pathological Aging. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019 Mar 29;68(2):679–93. pmid:30883347
  23. 23. Moffat SD, Resnick SM. Effects of age on virtual environment place navigation and allocentric cognitive mapping. Behav Neurosci. 2002 Oct;116(5):851–9. pmid:12369805
  24. 24. Serino S, Cipresso P, Morganti F, Riva G. The role of egocentric and allocentric abilities in Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review. Ageing Res Rev. 2014 Jul;16:32–44. pmid:24943907
  25. 25. Guariglia CC, Nitrini R. Topographical disorientation in Alzheimer’s disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2009 Dec;67(4):967–72. pmid:20069202
  26. 26. Aguirre GK D’Esposito M. Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. Brain. 1999 Sep 1;122(9):1613–28. pmid:10468502
  27. 27. Cerman J, Andel R, Laczo J, Vyhnalek M, Nedelska Z, Mokrisova I, et al. Subjective Spatial Navigation Complaints—A Frequent Symptom Reported by Patients with Subjective Cognitive Decline, Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2018 Jan 23;15(3):219–28. pmid:29165083
  28. 28. Lopez A, Caffò AO, Bosco A. Topographical disorientation in aging. Familiarity with the environment does matter. Neurol Sci. 2018 Sep;39(9):1519–28. pmid:29948464
  29. 29. Kempermann G, Fabel K, Ehninger D, Babu H, Leal-Galicia P, Garthe A, et al. Why and How Physical Activity Promotes Experience-Induced Brain Plasticity. Front Neurosci. 2010 Dec 8;4:189. pmid:21151782
  30. 30. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study. Psychol Sci. 2003 Mar 1;14(2):125–30. pmid:12661673
  31. 31. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008 Jan;9(1):58–65. pmid:18094706
  32. 32. Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Hoffman BM, Cooper H, Strauman TA, Welsh-Bohmer K, et al. Aerobic Exercise and Neurocognitive Performance: A Meta-Analytic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Psychosom Med. 2010 Apr;72(3):239–52. pmid:20223924
  33. 33. Cotman C, Berchtold . Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2002 Jun 1;25(6):295–301. pmid:12086747
  34. 34. Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie LA. Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends Neurosci. 2007 Sep;30(9):464–72. pmid:17765329
  35. 35. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011 Feb 15;108(7):3017–22. pmid:21282661
  36. 36. Griffin ÉW, Mullally S, Foley C, Warmington SA, O’Mara SM, Kelly ÁM. Aerobic exercise improves hippocampal function and increases BDNF in the serum of young adult males. Physiol Behav. 2011 Oct;104(5):934–41. pmid:21722657
  37. 37. Bo W, Lei M, Tao S, Jie LT, Qian L, Lin FQ, et al. Effects of combined intervention of physical exercise and cognitive training on cognitive function in stroke survivors with vascular cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil. 2019 Jan;33(1):54–63. pmid:30064268
  38. 38. Heisz JJ, Clark IB, Bonin K, Paolucci EM, Michalski B, Becker S, et al. The Effects of Physical Exercise and Cognitive Training on Memory and Neurotrophic Factors. J Cogn Neurosci. 2017 Nov;29(11):1895–907. pmid:28699808
  39. 39. Law LLF, Barnett F, Yau MK, Gray MA. Effects of combined cognitive and exercise interventions on cognition in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic review. Ageing Res Rev. 2014 May;15:61–75. pmid:24632497
  40. 40. Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet. 2015 Jun;385(9984):2255–63.
  41. 41. Styliadis C, Kartsidis P, Paraskevopoulos E, Ioannides AA, Bamidis PD. Neuroplastic Effects of Combined Computerized Physical and Cognitive Training in Elderly Individuals at Risk for Dementia: An eLORETA Controlled Study on Resting States. Neural Plast. 2015;2015:1–12. pmid:25945260
  42. 42. Gavelin HM, Dong C, Minkov R, Bahar-Fuchs A, Ellis KA, Lautenschlager NT, et al. Combined physical and cognitive training for older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Ageing Res Rev. 2021 Mar;66:101232. pmid:33249177
  43. 43. Seiler R. Cognitive Processes in Orienteering: A Review. Sci J Orienteer. 1996;12(2):50–65.
  44. 44. Eccles DW, Walsh SE, Ingledew DK. A grounded thoery of expert cognition in orienteering. J Sport Exerc Pyschology. 2002;24:68–88.
  45. 45. Eccles DW, Walsh SE, Ingledew DK. Visual attention in orienteers at different levels of experience. J Sports Sci. 2006 Jan;24(1):77–87. pmid:16368616
  46. 46. Feraco T, Bonvento M, Meneghetti C. Orienteering: What relation with visuospatial abilities, wayfinding attitudes, and environment learning? Appl Cogn Psychol. 2021 Nov;35(6):1592–9.
  47. 47. Roca-González C, Martin Gutierrez J, GarcÍa-Dominguez M, Mato Carrodeguas M del C. Virtual Technologies to Develop Visual-Spatial Ability in Engineering Students. EURASIA J Math Sci Technol Educ. 2016 Dec 2;13(2).
  48. 48. Bao S, Liu J, Liu Y. Shedding Light on the Effects of Orienteering Exercise on Spatial Memory Performance in College Students of Different Genders: An fNIRS Study. Brain Sci. 2022 Jun 29;12(7):852. pmid:35884661
  49. 49. International Alliance for Responsible Drinking (IARD). Drinking Guidelines: General Population. 2022. Available from: https://iard.org/science-resources/detail/Drinking-Guidelines-General-Population.
  50. 50. Fischer B, Russell C, Sabioni P, van den Brink W, Le Foll B, Hall W, et al. Lower-Risk Cannabis Use Guidelines: A Comprehensive Update of Evidence and Recommendations. Am J Public Health. 2017 Aug;107(8):e1–12. pmid:28644037
  51. 51. Mottet M, Saury J. Accurately locating one’s spatial position in one’s environment during a navigation task: Adaptive activity for finding or setting control flags in orienteering. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2013 Mar;14(2):189–99.
  52. 52. Palombo DJ, Williams LJ, Abdi H, Levine B. The survey of autobiographical memory (SAM): A novel measure of trait mnemonics in everyday life. Cortex. 2013 Jun;49(6):1526–40. pmid:23063319
  53. 53. Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology. Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology-Physical Activity Training for Health (CSEP-PATH). 2013. Available from: https://store.csep.ca/collections/csep-path.
  54. 54. Robin J, Buchsbaum BR, Moscovitch M. The Primacy of Spatial Context in the Neural Representation of Events. J Neurosci. 2018 Mar 14;38(11):2755–65. pmid:29440386
  55. 55. Konishi K, Bohbot VD. Spatial navigational strategies correlate with gray matter in the hippocampus of healthy older adults tested in a virtual maze. Front Aging Neurosci. 2013;5. pmid:23430962
  56. 56. Kovacevic A, Fenesi B, Paolucci E, Heisz JJ. The effects of aerobic exercise intensity on memory in older adults. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2020 Jun;45(6):591–600. pmid:31665610
  57. 57. Coutrot A, Silva R, Manley E, de Cothi W, Sami S, Bohbot VD, et al. Global Determinants of Navigation Ability. Curr Biol. 2018 Sep;28(17):2861–2866.e4. pmid:30100340
  58. 58. Segen V, Avraamides M, Slattery T, Colombo G, Wiener JM. Comparable performance on a spatial memory task in data collected in the lab and online. Castro-Alonso JC, editor. PLOS ONE. 2021 Nov 29;16(11):e0259367. pmid:34843521
  59. 59. Volz-Sidiropoulou E, Gauggel S. Do subjective measures of attention and memory predict actual performance? Metacognition in older couples. Psychol Aging. 2012 Jun;27(2):440–50. pmid:21942900
  60. 60. Hildenbrand L, Sanchez CA. Metacognitive accuracy across cognitive and physical task domains. Psychon Bull Rev. 2022 Aug;29(4):1524–30. pmid:35199328
  61. 61. Brunec IK, Robin J, Patai EZ, Ozubko JD, Javadi A, Barense MD, et al. Cognitive mapping style relates to posterior–anterior hippocampal volume ratio. Hippocampus. 2019 Aug;29(8):748–54. pmid:30714271
  62. 62. Liu I, Levy RM, Barton JJS, Iaria G. Age and gender differences in various topographical orientation strategies. Brain Res. 2011 Sep;1410:112–9. pmid:21803342
  63. 63. Sacco K, Ronga I, Perna P, Cicerale A, Del Fante E, Sarasso P, et al. A Virtual Navigation Training Promotes the Remapping of Space in Allocentric Coordinates: Evidence From Behavioral and Neuroimaging Data. Front Hum Neurosci. 2022 Apr 5;16:693968. pmid:35479185
  64. 64. Lövdén M, Schaefer S, Noack H, Bodammer NC, Kühn S, Heinze HJ, et al. Spatial navigation training protects the hippocampus against age-related changes during early and late adulthood. Neurobiol Aging. 2012 Mar;33(3):620.e9-620.e22. pmid:21497950
  65. 65. Wenger E, Schaefer S, Noack H, Kühn S, Mårtensson J, Heinze HJ, et al. Cortical thickness changes following spatial navigation training in adulthood and aging. NeuroImage. 2012 Feb;59(4):3389–97. pmid:22108645